Talking about Evolution with Richard
Dawkins
host Ben Wattenberg interviews
Richard Dawkins
Think Tank, http://www.pbs.org/
MR. WATTENBERG: Hello, I'm Ben Wattenberg.
Most Americans believe that Charles Darwin basically had it right,
that human beings evolved from the so-called primordial soup.
But most Americans arealso religious and likely believe that God
created the soup.
We will explore these ideas and others
with an outstanding scientist and one of the world's leading scientific
popularizers. The topic before this house: Richard Dawkins on
evolution and religion. This week on 'Think Tank.'
Richard Dawkins is a professor at OxfordUniversity,
where he holds the Charles Simone chair of public understanding
of science. Dawkins has written many books on thetopic of evolution,
including 'The Selfish Gene,' 'River Out of Eden,' 'The Blind
Watchmaker,' and most recently, 'Climbing MountImprobable.'
Dawkins' writings champion one man --
Charles Darwin. In 1831,Darwin set out on a five-year journey
around the world on the H.M.S.Beagle. His travels took him to
the Galapagos Islands off the coastof Ecuador, where he catalogued
a startling variety of plant andanimal life. Darwin saw in such
diversity the key to the origins ofall life on earth.
Today naturalists estimate that there
are 30 million species of plants and animals. According to Darwin's
theory, all creatures large and small are the end result of millions
of years of natural selection.
The reaction to Darwin's theory was explosive.
Critics declared that Darwin had replaced Adam with an ape. Atheists
applauded. Benjamin Disraeli, the prime minister of England, summed
up the debate at the time. He said, 'The question is, is man an
ape or an angel? Many laugh. Now I am on the side of the angels.'
Today the controversy persists. Evolution
is generally accepted, religion endures, begging the question,
is there a conflict?
Professor Dawkins, welcome. Perhaps we
could begin with that fascinating title, 'Climbing Mount Improbable.'
What are you talking about?
MR. DAWKINS: Living organisms are supremely
improbable. They look as if they have been designed. They are
very, very complicated. They are very good at doing whatever it
is they do, whether it's flying or digging or swimming. This is
not the kind of thing that matter just spontaneously does. It
doesn't fall into position where it's good at doing anything.
So the fact that living things are demands an explanation, the
fact that it's improbably demands an explanation.
Mount Improbable is a metaphorical mountain.
The height of that mountain stands for that very improbability.
So on the top of the mountain, you can imagine perched the most
complicated organ you can think of. It might be the human eye.
And one side of the mountain has a steep cliff, a steep vertical
precipice. And you stand at the foot of the mountain and you gaze
up at this complicated thing at the heights, and you say, that
couldn't have come about by chance, that's too improbable. And
that's what is the meaning of the vertical slope. You could no
more get that by sheer chance than you could leap from the bottom
of the cliff to the top of the cliff in one fell swoop.
But if you go around the other side of
the mountain, you find that there's not a steep cliff at all.
There's a slow, gentle gradient, a slow, gentle slope, and getting
from the bottom of the mountain to the top is an easy walk. You
just saunter up it putting one step in front of the other, one
foot in front of the other.
MR. WATTENBERG: Provided you have a billion
years to do it.
MR. DAWKINS: You've got to have a long
time. That, of course corresponds to Darwinian natural selection.
There is an element of chance in it, but it's not mostly chance.
There's a whole series of small chance steps. Each eye along the
slope is a little bit better than the one before, but it's not
so much that it's unbelievable that it could have come about by
chance. But at the end of a long period of non-random natural
selection, you've accumulated lots and lots of these steps, and
the end product is far too improbable to have come about in a
single step of chance.
MR. WATTENBERG: One of your earlier books,
a very well known book, is 'The Selfish Gene.' What does that
mean? You call human beings 'selfish gene machines.' Is that...
MR. DAWKINS: Yes. It's a way of trying
to explain why individual organisms like human beings are actually
not selfish. So I'm saying that selfishness resides at the level
of the gene. Genes that work for their own short-term survival,
genes that have effects upon the world which lead to their own
short-term survival are the genes that survive, the genes that
come through the generations. The world is full of genes that
look after their own selfish interest.
MR. WATTENBERG: And the prime aspect of
that is reproduction?
MR. DAWKINS: Yes.
MR. WATTENBERG: And so that's what drives
all organisms, including human beings, is the drive to reproduce
their own genetic makeup?
MR. DAWKINS: That's pretty standard Darwinism.
MR. WATTENBERG: Right.
MR. DAWKINS: We are in any era - the organisms
that live - contain the genes of an unbroken line of successful
ancestors. I thas to be true. Plenty of the ancestors' competitors
were not successful. They all died. But not a single one of your
ancestors died young, or not a single one of your ancestors failed
to copulate not a single one of your ancestors failed to rear
at least one child.
MR. WATTENBERG: By definition.
MR. DAWKINS: By definition. And so --
but what's not by definition, which is genuinely interesting,
is that you have therefore inherited the genes which are a non-random
sample of the genes in every generation, non-random in the direction
of being good at surviving.
MR. WATTENBERG: What is motivating great
musicians, greatwriters, great political leaders, great scientists?
I mean, what are you doing now? You're obviously passionate about
what you write and what you think and what you're doing. That
is absorbing your life. That does not involve, I don't think,
the replication of your genetic makeup.
MR. DAWKINS: That's certainly right, and
because we are humans, we tend to be rather obsessed with humans.
There are 30 million other species of animal where that question
wouldn't have occurred to you.
MR. WATTENBERG: Yeah, but most of our
viewers are humans. Now, how does that work out for -- are humans
different?
MR. DAWKINS: Humans, like any other species
of animal, have been programmed -- have evolved by genetic selection.
And we havethe bodies and the brains that are good for passing
on our genes. That's step one. So that's where we get our brains
from. That's why they're big.
But once you get a big brain, then the
big brain can be used for other things, in the same sort of way
as computers were originally designed as calculating machines,
and then without any change, without any alteration of that general
structure, it turnsout that they're good -- they can be used as
word processors as well. So there's something about human brains
which makes them more versatile than they were originally intended
for.
Now, you talked about the fact that I'm
passionate about what Ido and that I work hard at writing my books
and so on. Now, the way I would interpret that as a Darwinian
is to say certainly writing books doesn't increase your Darwinian
fitness. Writing books --there are no genes for writing books,
and certainly I don't pass on any of my genes as a consequence
of writing a book.
But there are mechanisms, such as persistence,
perseverance, setting up goals which you then work hard to achieve,
driving yourself to achieve those goals by whatever means are
available.
MR. WATTENBERG: And you believe that is
in our genetic makeup?
MR. DAWKINS: That's what I believe is
indicated.
MR. WATTENBERG: Some people have more
of it, some people have less of it.
MR. DAWKINS: That's right. Now, in the
modern world, which is now so different from the world in which
our ancestors lived, what we actually strive for, the goals we
set up, are very different. The goal-seeking mechanisms in our
brains were originally put there to try to achieve goals such
as finding a herd of bison to hunt. And we would have set out
to find a herd of bison, and we'd have used all sorts of flexible
goal-seeking mechanisms and we'd have persisted and we'd have
gone on and on and on for days and days and days trying to achieve
that goal.
Natural selection favored persistence
in seeking goals. Nowadays we no longer hunt bisons. Nowadays
we hunt money or a nice new house or we try to finish a novel
or whatever it is that we do.
MR. WATTENBERG: In this town, political
victory.
MR. DAWKINS: Yes, right.
MR. WATTENBERG: Why is this so important?
I mean, you obviously feel that this idea of evolution of primary
importance. I mean, this is what makes the world goes round. Is
it, in your view at least, the mother science?
MR. DAWKINS: Well, what could be more
important than an understanding of why you're here, why you're
the shape you are, why you have the brain that you do, why your
body is the way it is. Not just you, but all the other 30 million
species of living thing, each of which carries with it this superb
illusion of having been designed to do something supremely well.
A swift flies supremely well. A mole digs supremely well. A shark
or a dolphin swims supremely well. And a human thinks supremely
well.
What could be a more fascinating, tantalizing
question than why all that has come about? And we have the answer.
Since the middle of the 19th century, we have known in principle
the answer to that question, and we're still working out the details.
MR. WATTENBERG: Well, I read that, and
a long time ago I read some of Darwin. Darwin doesn't really answer
the question why we are here. He answers the question of how we
are here. I mean, why in a-- when you normally say, well, why
are we here, you expect a theological answer or a religious answer.
Does Darwin really talk about why we are here in that sense?
MR. DAWKINS: Darwin, if I may say so,
had better things to do than talk about why we are here in that
sense. It's not a sensible sense in which to ask the question.
There is no reason why, just because it's possible to ask the
question, it's necessarily a sensible question to ask.
MR. WATTENBERG: But you had mentioned,
you said that Darwina fter all these years has told us why we're
here.
MR. DAWKINS: I was using 'why' in another
sense. I was using 'why' in the sense of the explanation, and
that's the only sense which I think is actually a legitimate one.
I don't think the question of ultimate purpose, the question of
what is the fundamental purpose for which the universe came into
existence -- I believe there isn't one. If you asked me what --
MR. WATTENBERG: You believe there is not
one?
MR. DAWKINS: Yes. On the other hand, if
you ask me, what is the purpose of a bird's wing, then I'm quite
happy to say, well, in the special Darwinian sense, the purpose
of a bird's wing is to help it fly, therefore to survive and therefore
to reproduce the genes that gave it those wings that make it fly.
Now, I'm happy with that meaning of the
word 'why'.
MR. WATTENBERG: I see.
MR. DAWKINS: But the ultimate meaning
of the word 'why' I do not regard as a legitimate question. And
the mere fact that it's possible to ask the question doesn't make
it legitimate. There are plenty of questions I could imagine somebody
asking me and I wouldn't attempt to answer it. I would just say,
That's a silly question, don't ask it.
MR. WATTENBERG: So you are not only saying
that religious people are coming to a wrong conclusion. You are
saying they're asking a silly question.
MR. DAWKINS: Yes.
MR. WATTENBERG: There is a scientist in
the United States named Michael Beahy -- I'm sure you're involved
in this argument --who is making the case -- he is not a creationist,
he is not a creation scientist, or at least he says he's --
MR. DAWKINS: Well, I'm sorry, he is a
creationist.
MR. WATTENBERG: Well, he says he's not.
MR. DAWKINS: He says he's not, but he
is.
MR. WATTENBERG: He says he's not. But
his theory is that of a hidden designer, that there is something
driving this process. And could you explain how you and he differ
on this?
MR. DAWKINS: Yes. Like I said, he's a
creationist. 'A hidden designer,' that's a creator.
MR. WATTENBERG: You say he's a hidden
creationist.
MR. DAWKINS: Well, he's not even hidden.
He's a straightforward creationist. What he has done is to take
a standard argument which dates back to the 19th century, the
argument of irreducible complexity, the argument that there are
certain organs, certain systems in which all the bits have to
be there together or the whole system won't work.
MR. WATTENBERG: Like the eye.
MR. DAWKINS: Like the eye, right. The
whole thing collapses if they're not all there.
Now, Darwin considered that argument for
the eye and he dismissed it, correctly, by showing that actually
the eye could hav eevolved by gradual stages. Bits of an eye --
half an eye is better than no eye, a quarter of an eye is better
than no eye, half an eye is better than a quarter of an eye.
MR. WATTENBERG: I mean if it has some
sight, but if you just created the windshield wiper, it doesn't
--
MR. DAWKINS: Exactly. So I mean, there
are things which youc ould imagine which are irreducibly complex,
but the eye is not one of them.
Now, Beahy is saying, well, maybe the
eye isn't one of them, but at the molecular level, there are certain
things which he says are. Now, he takes certain molecular examples.
For example, bacteria have a flagellum, which is a little kind
of whip-like tail by which they swim. And the flagellum is a remarkable
thing because, uniquely in all the living kingdoms, it's a true
wheel. It actually rotates freely in a bearing; it has an axle
which freely rotates. That's a remarkable thing and is well understood
and well known about.
And Beahy asserts: this is irreducibly
complex, therefore God made it. Now --
MR. WATTENBERG: Therefore there was a
design to it. I don't think --
MR. DAWKINS: What's the difference? Okay.
MR. WATTENBERG: Whoa.
MR. DAWKINS: Therefore there was a design
to it.
MR. WATTENBERG: Right.
MR. DAWKINS: Now -- (audio gap) -- too
complex. The eye is reducibly complex, therefore God made it.
Darwin answered them point by point, piece by piece. But maybe
he shouldn't have bothered. Maybe what he should have said is,
well, maybe you can't think of --maybe you're too thick to think
of a reason why the eye could have come about by gradual steps,
but perhaps you should go away and think a bit harder.
Now, I've done it for the eye; I've done
it for various other things. I haven't yet done it for the bacterial
flagellum. I've only just read Beahy's book. It's an interesting
point. I'd like to think about it.
But I'm not the best person equipped to
think about it because I'm not a biochemist. You've got to have
the equivalent biochemical knowledge to the knowledge that Darwin
had about lenses and bits ofeyes. Now, I don't have that biochemical
knowledge. Beahy has.
Beahy should stop being lazy and should
get up and think for himself about how the flagellum evolved instead
of this cowardly, lazy copping out by simply saying, oh, I can't
think of how it came about, therefore it must have been designed.
MR. WATTENBERG: You have written that
being an atheist allows you to become intellectually fulfilled.
MR. DAWKINS: No, I haven't quite written
that. What I have written is that before Darwin, it was difficult
to be an intellectually fulfilled atheist and that Darwin made
it easy to become an intellectually -- and it's more. It's more.
If you wanted to be an atheist, it would have been hard to be
an atheist before Darwin came along. But once Darwin came along,
the argument from design, which has always been to me the only
powerful argument --even that isn't a very powerful argument,
but I used to think it was the only powerful argument for the
existence of a creator.
Darwin destroyed the argument from design,
at least as far as biology is concerned, which has always been
the happiest hunting ground for argument from design. Thereafter
-- whereas before Darwin came along, you could have been an atheist,
but you'd have been a bit worried, after Darwin you can be an
intellectually fulfilled atheist. You can feel, really, now I
understand how living things have acquired the illusion of design,
I understand why they look as though they've been designed, whereas
before Darwin came along, you'd have said, well, I can see that
the theory of a divine creator isn't a good theory, but I'm damned
if I can think of a better one. After Darwin, you can think of
a better one.
MR. WATTENBERG: I mean, isn't the standard
rebuttal to that that God created Darwin and He could have created
this whole evolutionary illusion that you are talking about? And
I mean, getting back to first causes that you sort of --
MR. DAWKINS: Yes. Yeah. Not that God created
Darwin, but you mean God created the conditions in which evolution
happened.
MR. WATTENBERG: And Darwin.
MR. DAWKINS: Well, ultimately Darwin,
too.
MR. WATTENBERG: I mean ultimately.
MR. DAWKINS: Yes, it's not a very satisfying
explanation. It's a very unparsimonious, very uneconomical explanation.
The beauty of the Darwinian explanation itself is that it's exceedingly
powerful. It's a very simple principle, and using this one simple
principle, you can bootstrap your way up from essentially nothing
to the world of complexity and diversity we have today. Now, that's
apowerful explanation.
MR. WATTENBERG: It's not any simpler.
In fact, it's morecomplex than the -- than Genesis. I mean, 'And
God created the heavens and the earth.' That --
MR. DAWKINS: You have to be joking.
MR. WATTENBERG: Well, I mean, 'God created
the heavens and the earth' -- I can say that pretty quickly. I
mean --
MR. DAWKINS: You can say it, but think
what lies behind it. What lies behind it is a complicated, intelligent
being -- God, who must have come from somewhere. You have simply
smuggled in at the beginning of your book the very thing that
we're trying to explain. What we're trying to explain is where
organized complexity and intelligence came from. We have now got
an explanation. You start from nothing and you work up gradually
in easily explainable steps.
MR. WATTENBERG: But then I can ask you
the same question: where does the nothing come from? I mean, this
is a -- I mean, I don't want this to degenerate into a sophomore
beer brawl, but I mean, you know, that is -- isn't that the ultimate
--
MR. DAWKINS: You can ask that. That's
the ultimate question.
MR. WATTENBERG: Right.
MR. DAWKINS: That's the important question.
But all I would say to that is that it's a helluva lot easier
to say where nothing came from than it is to say where 30 million
species of highly complicated organisms plus a superintelligent
God came from, and that's the alternative.
MR. WATTENBERG: Well, now, you wrote in
'The Selfish Gene' this. 'Living organisms had existed on earth
without ever knowing why for 3,000 million years before the truth
finally dawned on one of them. His name was Charles Darwin.'
That sounds to me like a religious statement.
That is a --that is near messianic language. And you are making
the case that these other people have this virus of the mind.
That tonality says, I found my God.
MR. DAWKINS: You can call it that if you
like. It's not religious in any sense in which I would recognize
the term. Certainly I look up to Charles Darwin. I would look
up to anybody who had the insight that he did. But I wasn't really
meaning to make a particularly messianic statement about Darwin.
I was rather saying that not just Darwin,
but this species, homo sapiens -- or for the -- the time that
has elapsed between the origin of humanity and Darwin is negligible
compared to the time that elapsed from the origin of life and
the origin of humanity. And so let's modify that statement and
make it a bit more universal and say, life has been going on this
planet for 3,000 million years without any animals knowing why
they were there until the truth finally dawned upon homo sapiens.
It's just happened to be Charles Darwin, it could have been somebody
else.
Our species is unique. We are all members
of a unique species which is privileged to understand for the
first time in that 3,000-million-year history why we are here.
MR. WATTENBERG: I see. There was a study
recently reported, I believe, in that great scientific journal
'USA Today,' but it's one that had a certain resonance with me
and I think other people. It said that people who are religious
live longer and healthier lives. And it seems to me on its face,
perhaps to you as well, that that makes some sense. I mean, people
who do have a firm belief system and don't worry about a whole
lot of things are healthier. We've seen this in all the mind-body
sorts of explorations that have beengoing on.
But does that perhaps put a Darwinian
bonus on believing in religion?
MR. DAWKINS: It could well do, yes. It's
perfectly plausible to me. I've read the same study and I think
it might well be true. It could be analogous to the placebo effect,
you know, that many diseases -- obviously they're cured by real
medicines even better, but nevertheless if you give people a pill
which doesn't contain anything medicinal at all, but the patient
believes it does, then the patient gets better, for some diseases.
Well, I suppose that religious belief
can be one big placebo and it could indeed have highly beneficial
effects upon health, particularly where stress-related diseases
are concerned.
MR. WATTENBERG: So if I want to advise
my viewers, I could say, for example, what Professor Dawkins says
is true, but harmful; I would like you to believe something that's
false, and healthy.
MR. DAWKINS: Yeah, you could say that.
I mean, it depends whether you value health or truth better, more.
MR. WATTENBERG: Which would you value?
MR. DAWKINS: For myself, I would rather
live a little bit less long and know the truth about why I live
rather than live a few -- it probably isn't very much longer,
actually, which is -- let's be very...
MR. WATTENBERG: Suppose it was substantially
longer and we were talking about your children rather than you.
MR. DAWKINS: Yeah, okay. I mean, these
are fascinating hypothetical questions and I suppose there would
come a trade-off point. I mean, there'd probably come a point
when -- but I do think it's important, since this is a very academic
discussion we're having, I think it would be positively irresponsible
to let listeners to this program go away with the idea that this
is a major effect. If it's an effect at all, it's an elusive statistical
effect.
MR. WATTENBERG: Okay, thank you very much,
Professor Richard Dawkins.
MR. DAWKINS: Thank you.
Richard
Dawkins page
Home Page